Psychology and Behavioral Sciences

| Peer-Reviewed |

Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students

Received: 15 March 2021    Accepted: 29 March 2021    Published: 16 April 2021
Views:       Downloads:

Share This Article

Abstract

A group of dyslexics with auditory processing difficulties showed significant improvements in language skills following intervention using non-linguistic auditory stimulation to enhance sensitivity and obtain a ‘healthy’ right ear advantage. Twenty–eight participants aged thirteen to seventeen years were divided into three groups: a dyslexic intervention group, a dyslexic control group and a non-dyslexic control group. The intervention used was Johansen Individualised Auditory Stimulation (Johansen IAS). The intervention group listened individually for ten minutes daily over fifteen to eighteen months to CDs of computer-generated music customised according to the results of their hearing tests. Improvements in technical reading (decoding) and spelling abilities in the dyslexic intervention group support a link between basic sensory perception skills and language-related skills at a phonological level. The study supports the use of non-linguistic auditory stimulation to optimise auditory perception, and the notion that such interventions benefit language in dyslexics whose auditory sensitivity and laterality is atypical. Further research is suggested to investigate the link between fundamental auditory processing abilities and our ability to learn and process language. The importance of assessing basic auditory perception, and the potential for its ‘re-education’ to optimise phonological awareness (widely accepted as a crucial process in literacy) is highlighted.

DOI 10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12
Published in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences (Volume 10, Issue 2, April 2021)
Page(s) 81-95
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Auditory Stimulation, Developmental Dyslexia, Improving Language Abilities, Phonological Awareness, Johansen IAS, Music

References
[1] Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading retardness. Nature, 271, 5647, 746-747.
[2] Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[3] Snowling, Margaret J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research 43: 219-234.
[4] Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study. Brain, 126; 841-865.
[5] Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1; 329-342.
[6] Paulesu, E, J. F. Démonet, F. Fazio, S. F. Cappa, G. Cossu, C. D. Frith, and U. Frith. (2000) A cultural effect on brain function. Natural Neuroscience, 3, 91-96.
[7] Grigorenko, E. (2001) Developmental dyslexia: an update on genes, brains and environments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 91-125.
[8] Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1993). Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the pre-eminence of temporal processing. In P. Tallal, A. M. Galaburda, R. R. Llinas, & C. von Euler, (eds.), Temporal information processing in the nervous system (Vol. 82, pp. 27-47). New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[9] Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits in language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77-81.
[10] Tallal, P. (2000) The science of literacy: from the laboratory to the classroom. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
[11] Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2002). Deficits in phoneme awareness do not arise from failures in rapid auditory processing, Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 5-14.
[12] Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Deutsch, G., Tallal, P., and Temple, E. (2007). Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: An fMRI study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 25, 295-310.
[13] Clark, A. D. and Richards, C. J. (1966). Auditory discrimination among economically disadvantaged and non-disadvantaged preschool children. Except. Child. 33: 259-62.
[14] Benasich, A. A., Thomas, J. J. and Choudbury, N. (2002). The Importance of Rapid Auditory Processing Abilities to Early Language Development: Evidence from Converging Methodologies. Developmental Psychobiology, 40 (3), 278-292.
[15] Wright, B. A., Lombardino, L. J., King, W. M., Puranick, C. S., Leonard, C. M. & Merzenich, M. M. (1997). Defecits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children. Nature, 387, 176-178.
[16] Boets B., Wouters J., Van Wieringen A. & Ghesquiere P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 8, 1608-1620.
[17] McCall, R. B. & Plemons, B. W. (2001). The concept of critical periods and their implications for early childhood services. In D. B. Bailey, Jr., J. Bruer, F. J. Symons, & J. W. Lichtman (Eds.), Critical thinking about critical periods (pp 267-287). Baltimore: Paul Brookes.
[18] Friederici, A. D. & Alter, K. (2004). Lateralization of auditory language functions: A dynamic dual pathway model. Brain and Language, 89, 267-276.
[19] Musiek, F., Kibbe, K., & Baran, J. (1984). Neuroaudiological results from split-brain patients. Seminars in Hearing, 5, 219-229.
[20] Knecht, S. Flöel, A. Dräger, B, Breitenstein, C, Sommer, J. Henningsen, H. Ringelstein, E. B. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2002) Degree of language lateralization determines susceptibility to unilateral brain lesions. Nature Neuroscience 5, 695-699.
[21] Broca, P. (1861). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Bulletin de la Société Anatomique, 6, 330-350.
[22] Pujol J, Deus J, Losilla JM, Capdevila A. (1999) Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology 1999; 52: 1038–43.
[23] Wernicke, C. (1874). Der Aphasiche Symptomen-komplex. Breslau: Cohn and Weigert. Republished as: The aphasia symptom complex: A psychological study on an anatomical basis. Wernicke’s works on aphasia. The Hague: Mouton.
[24] Corina D. P, Vaid J. & Bellugi U. (1992). The linguistic basis of left hemisphere specialization. Science, Vol 255, Issue 5049, 1258-1260.
[25] Levanen, S. Uutela, K. Salenius, S. and Hari, R. (2001) Cortical Representation of Sign Language: Comparison of Deaf Signers and Hearing Non-signers. Cerebral Cortex 11, 506-512.
[26] Sininger Y. S., Cone-Wesson B. (2004). Asymmetric Cochlear Processing Mimics Hemispheric Specialisation. Science, 305, no. 5690, 1581.
[27] Mills, D. L., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. J. (1997). Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 397–445.
[28] Helland, T., Asbjørnsen, A. E., Hushovd, A. E. and Hugdahl, K. (2008). Dichotic Listening and School Performance in Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14, 42-53.
[29] Näslund, J. C., Johansen, K. V., and Thoma, R. 1997. Predicting Decoding Level from Dichotic Listening. Paper presented at the 4th Annual Meeting in the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, Boston March 23-24, 1997.
[30] Moncrieff, D. W., Musiek, F. E. (2002) Interaural asymmetries revealed by dichotic listening tests in normal and dyslexic children. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 13, 428-437.
[31] Yarrow, K., Brown, P. & Krakauer, J. W. (2009) Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 585-596.
[32] Pascual-Leone A, Nguyet D, Cohen LG, Brasil-Neto JP, Cammarota A, Hallett M. (1995) Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (3): 1037-45.
[33] Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B. and Taub, E. (1995), 'Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players', Science, 270, 5234, 305-7.
[34] Hyde K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C. & Sclaug, G. (2009) Musical Training Shapes Structural Brain Development. The Journal of Neuroscience; 29 (10: 3019-3025).
[35] Leviton, A. and Bellinger, D. (1986). Is There a Relationship between Otitis Media and Learning Disorder. In J. F. Kavanagh: Otitis Media and Child Development. Baltimore: York Press.
[36] Freeark, K., Frank, S. J., Wagner, A. E., Lopez, M., Olmsted, C. & Girard, R. (1992). Otitis Meda, Language Development, ad Parental Verbal Stimulation. Journal of Paediatric Psychology, 17 (2), 173-185.
[37] Bruer, J. T., & Greenough, W. T. (2001). The subtle science of how experience affects the brain. In D. B. Bailey, Jr., J. Bruer, F. J. Symons, & J. W. Lichtman (Eds.), Critical thinking about critical periods (pp. 209-232). Baltimore: Paul Brookes.
[38] Whitlaw, G. M. and Yuskow, K (2006) Neuromaturation and neuroplasticity of the central auditory system. In Parthasarathy TK (ed), An Introduction to Auditory Processing Disorders in Children (pp. 21-38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
[39] Cho Lieu, J. E. (2004). Speech-Language and Educational Consequences of Unilateral Hearing Loss in Children. Archives of Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, 130, 524-530.
[40] Jensen, J. H., Børre, S., and Johansen, P. A. (1989). Unilateral sensorineural hearing loss in children: cognitive abilities with respect to right/left ear differences. British Journal of Audiology, 23: 215-220.
[41] McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. (2004). Which people with specific language impairment have auditory processing deficits? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 79–94.
[42] Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, Vol. 31, 3-4, 509-527.
[43] Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). Auditory word identification and phonological skills in dyslexic and average readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 419-439.
[44] Griffiths, Y. M. & Snowling, M. J (2002) Predictors of exception word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: the severity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, (1), 34-43.
[45] Heath SM, Hogben JH, Clark CD. (1999). Auditory temporal processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40: 637–47.
[46] Hill NI, Bailey PJ, Griffiths YM, Snowling MJ. (1999). Frequency acuity and binaural masking release in dyslexic listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 1999; 106: L53–8.
[47] Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G. et al (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-84.
[48] Treharne, D. (2001) Efficacy of TLP with Children with Auditory Processing Problems. Paper presented at TLP conference November 2001.
[49] Musiek, F. & Schochat, E. (1998). Auditory training and central auditory processing disorders: A case study. Seminars in Hearing, 19, 357-366.
[50] Tallal, P., Stark, R., Kallman, C., & Mellits, D. (1981). A re-examination of some nonverbal perceptual abilities of language-impaired and normal children as a function of age and sensory modality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 351-357.
[51] Tallal, P., & Merzenich, M. Jenkins, W. Miller, S. And Peterson, B.(1997) Temporal Training for Language-Impaired Children: National Clinical Trial Results. Mini-seminar presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language Hearing Association, Boston MA.
[52] Moncrieff D. W. & Wertz D. (2001). Auditory rehabilitation for interaural asymmetry: Preliminary evidence of improved dichotic listening performance following intensive training. International Journal of Audiology, 47, 2, 84-97.
[53] Patel, A. D. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Natural Neuroscience, 6, 674-681.
[54] Koelsch, S. (2005). Neural substrates of processing syntax and semantics in music. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 15, 207-212.
[55] Miranda, R. A. & Ullman, M. T. (2007). Double dissociation between rules and memory in music: An event-related potential study. Neuroimage, 38, 331-345.
[56] Koelsch, S.; Gunter, T. C.; von Cramon, D. Y.; Zysset, S.; Lohmann, G.; Friederici, A. D. (2002). Bach speaks: A cortical "language-network" serves the processing of music. Neuroimage, 17 (2), 956-966.
[57] Forde Thompson W. E., Schellenberg, G. & Husain G. (2006) Perceiving Prosody in Speech-Effects of Music Lessons, in: The Neurosciences and Music II, New York Academy of Sciences.
[58] Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K. and Winner, E. (2005). Effects of music training on the child’s brain and cognitive development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060: 219-230.
[59] Treharne, D (2008) The relationship between rhythmic ability and language comprehension, Chap. 4 PhD thesis, University of Sheffield.
[60] Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E. Russo, N. M., Dees, T. & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 420-422.
[61] Musacchia G., Sams M., Skoe E., Kraus N. (2007). Musicians have enhanced subcortical auditory and audiovisual processing of speech and music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 15894-15898.
[62] Pannekamp, A., Toepel, U., Alter, K., Hahne, A. & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Prosody-driven sentence processing: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 (3), 407-421.
[63] Richardson, U., Thomson, J., Scott, S. K. & Goswami, U. (2004). Auditory processing skills and phonological representation in dyslexic children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 10 (3), 215-233.
[64] Wallace WT. (1994) Memory for music: Effect of melody on recall of tekst. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1994; 20 (6): 1471-1485.
[65] Bellis, T. J. (1996). Assessment and management of central auditory processing disorders in the educational setting from science to practice. San Diego, Calif: Singular Publishing.
[66] Gilmor, T. M. (1999), The Efficacy of the Tomatis method for Children with Learning and Communication Disorders, International Journal of Listening, 13, 12.
[67] Van Dijk & Tellegen. (1994). Groninger Intelligence test for Continued Education. Amsterdam / Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
[68] Brus, B. Th. & Voeten, M. J. M. (1972). One-minute-test; form A and B. Nijmegen: Berkhout.
[69] Van den Bos, K. P., H. C. Lutje Spelberg, A. J. M. Scheepstra & J. R. de Vries (1994). Non-word test Klepel; form A and B. Nijmegen: Berkhout.
[70] Henneman, K., Kleijnen, R., Smits, A. (2004). Silent Reading Test: How dangerous is a tick bite? In: Protocol Dyslexia Continued Education. ’s-Hertogenbosch, KPC groep / Nijmegen, Expertisecentrum Nederlands / Tilburg, Werkverband Opleidingen Speciaal Onderwijs.
[71] Hacquebord, H. I. (1996). Text comprehension test for seventh grade.`s Hertogenbosch: KPC-groep.
[72] Henneman, K., Kleijnen, R., Smits, A. (2004). Sentence dictation: The wonderful weather. In: Protocol Dyslexia Continued Education. ’s-Hertogenbosch, KPC groep / Nijmegen, Expertisecentrum Nederlands / Tilburg, Werkverband Opleidingen Speciaal Onderwijs.
[73] Henneman, K., Kleijnen, R., Smits, A. (2004). Writing / Copying task. In: Protocol Dyslexia Continued Education. ’s-Hertogenbosch, KPC groep / Nijmegen, Expertisecentrum Nederlands / Tilburg, Werkverband Opleidingen Speciaal Onderwijs.
[74] Schenk, M. N., T. van Luijn-Hindriks & G. Q. A. Nieuwenhoek. (1984). Digit span test. In: Remedial Teaching, a guidebook for educational support. Deventer: Van Loghum Slaterus.
[75] Schenk, M. N., T. van Luijn-Hindriks & G. Q. A. Nieuwenhoek. (1984). Test visual memory. In: Remedial Teaching, a guidebook for educational support. Deventer: Van Loghum Slaterus.
[76] Van Luijn-Hindriks, T. (1992). Phoneme Analysis Test. In: Speech and spelling exercises for the advanced speller. Twello: Van Tricht.
[77] Van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J. H.. Lutje Spelberg, H. C. (2000). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors and picture objects, and word reading speed. Scientific Studies of Reading.
[78] Merzenich, M. M., Schreiner C., Jenkins W., and Wang X. (1993). Neural mechanisms underlying temporal integration, segmentation, and input sequence representation: some implications for the origin of learning disabilities. Ann NY Acad Sci 682: 1–22.
[79] Bertucci C, Hook P, Haynes C, Macaruso P, & Bickley C (2003) Vowel Perception and production in adolescents with reading disabilities Annals of Dyslexia Volume 53, Number 1, 174-200, DOI: 10.1007/s11881-003-0009-1.
[80] Moncrieff DW, Black JR (2008) Dichotich listening deficits in children with dyslexia in, Dyslexia Volume 14, Issue 1, pages 54-75, February 2008.
[81] Plaza, M., Cohen, M. (2007) The contribution of phonological awareness and visual attention in early reading and spelling. Dyslexia, 13, 1, 67-76.4.
[82] Valdois, S., Bosse, M., Tainturier, M. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of the evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 4, 339-363.
Author Information
  • BC Broekhin, Johansen Individualised Auditory Stimulation (J-IAS), Herkenbosch, The Netherlands

Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Wim de Zwart. (2021). Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 10(2), 81-95. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Wim de Zwart. Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2021, 10(2), 81-95. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Wim de Zwart. Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students. Psychol Behav Sci. 2021;10(2):81-95. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12,
      author = {Wim de Zwart},
      title = {Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students},
      journal = {Psychology and Behavioral Sciences},
      volume = {10},
      number = {2},
      pages = {81-95},
      doi = {10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12},
      eprint = {https://download.sciencepg.com/pdf/10.11648.j.pbs.20211002.12},
      abstract = {A group of dyslexics with auditory processing difficulties showed significant improvements in language skills following intervention using non-linguistic auditory stimulation to enhance sensitivity and obtain a ‘healthy’ right ear advantage. Twenty–eight participants aged thirteen to seventeen years were divided into three groups: a dyslexic intervention group, a dyslexic control group and a non-dyslexic control group. The intervention used was Johansen Individualised Auditory Stimulation (Johansen IAS). The intervention group listened individually for ten minutes daily over fifteen to eighteen months to CDs of computer-generated music customised according to the results of their hearing tests. Improvements in technical reading (decoding) and spelling abilities in the dyslexic intervention group support a link between basic sensory perception skills and language-related skills at a phonological level. The study supports the use of non-linguistic auditory stimulation to optimise auditory perception, and the notion that such interventions benefit language in dyslexics whose auditory sensitivity and laterality is atypical. Further research is suggested to investigate the link between fundamental auditory processing abilities and our ability to learn and process language. The importance of assessing basic auditory perception, and the potential for its ‘re-education’ to optimise phonological awareness (widely accepted as a crucial process in literacy) is highlighted.},
     year = {2021}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Possible Effects of Specific Auditory Stimulation (Johansen-IAS) on Language Development of a Group of Dyslexic Students
    AU  - Wim de Zwart
    Y1  - 2021/04/16
    PY  - 2021
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12
    DO  - 10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12
    T2  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JF  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JO  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    SP  - 81
    EP  - 95
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2328-7845
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211002.12
    AB  - A group of dyslexics with auditory processing difficulties showed significant improvements in language skills following intervention using non-linguistic auditory stimulation to enhance sensitivity and obtain a ‘healthy’ right ear advantage. Twenty–eight participants aged thirteen to seventeen years were divided into three groups: a dyslexic intervention group, a dyslexic control group and a non-dyslexic control group. The intervention used was Johansen Individualised Auditory Stimulation (Johansen IAS). The intervention group listened individually for ten minutes daily over fifteen to eighteen months to CDs of computer-generated music customised according to the results of their hearing tests. Improvements in technical reading (decoding) and spelling abilities in the dyslexic intervention group support a link between basic sensory perception skills and language-related skills at a phonological level. The study supports the use of non-linguistic auditory stimulation to optimise auditory perception, and the notion that such interventions benefit language in dyslexics whose auditory sensitivity and laterality is atypical. Further research is suggested to investigate the link between fundamental auditory processing abilities and our ability to learn and process language. The importance of assessing basic auditory perception, and the potential for its ‘re-education’ to optimise phonological awareness (widely accepted as a crucial process in literacy) is highlighted.
    VL  - 10
    IS  - 2
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

  • Sections