| Peer-Reviewed

Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li

Received: 31 March 2017    Accepted: 20 April 2017    Published: 13 June 2017
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

This study examined the spontaneous language use between parents and their preschool children who are at high-risk for ADHD with a co-occurring language impairment (LI) and those without a co-occurring LI. Semantic and syntactic differences in language use were examined. Participants consisted of 20 children ages three to five years old and their parents. For each parent-child dyad, a 15-minute video-recorded interaction was orthographically transcribed and analyzed using the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software program. Children with co-occuring LI showed significantly lower Mean Length of Utterance (MLU), and Type-Token Ratios (TTR). Both groups showed very limited use of complex sentences and no specific difficulties with grammatical morphemes. Parents in both groups did not seem to adapt their language level whether or not their children had a LI.

Published in International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders (Volume 1, Issue 1)
DOI 10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15
Page(s) 22-29
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

ADHD, Language Impairment, Parent-Child Interaction, Preschool

References
[1] S. Visser, R. H. Danielson, R. H. Bitsko and J. R. & Holbrook, “Convergent validity of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis: A cross-sectional study comparison,” Annals of Epidemiology, vol. 23, pp. 592-601, 2013.
[2] Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, “Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Data and statistics”, retrieved from www.cdc.gpv/NCBDDD/adhd/data.html.
[3] B. B. Lahey, W. E. Pelham, J. Loney, H. Kipp, A. Ehrhardt, S. S. Lee and G. Massetti, “Three-year predictive validity of DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children diagnosed at 4–6 years of age,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 161, no. 11, pp. 2014-2020, 2004.
[4] M. Matos, J. J. Bauermeister and G. Bernal, “Parent-child interaction therapy for puerto rican preschool children with ADHD and behavior problems: a pilot efficacy study,” Family Process, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 232-252, 2009.
[5] E. J, Sonuga‐Barke, J. Auerbach, S. B. Campbell, D. Daley and M. Thompson, “Varieties of preschool hyperactivity: multiple pathways from risk to disorder,” Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 141-150, 2005.
[6] T. E. Wilens, J. Biederman and T. J. Spencer, “Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan,” Annual review of medicine, vol. 53, no. 1, pp. 113-131, 2002.
[7] N. J. Cohen, D. D. Vallance, M. Barwick, N. Im, R. Menna, N. B. Horodezkyand L. Isaacson, “The interface between ADHD and language impairment: an examination of language, achievement, and cognitive processing. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 353-362, 2000.
[8] S. M. Redmond, “Conversational profiles of children with ADHD, SLI and typical development,” Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 107–125, 2004.
[9] R. A. Barkley, Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press, 2006.
[10] T. E. Brown, Attention-deficit disorders and comorbidities in children, adolescents, and adults. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 2000.
[11] R. Paul and F. Nurbury, Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Assessment and intervention. St. Louis, MO; Mosby, Inc, 2012.
[12] NIH (National Institute of Mental Health), “Specific language impairment in children fact sheet,” NIH Publication, No. 11, p. 7751, 2011. Retrieved from http://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/voice/pages/specific-language-impairment.aspx.
[13] H. M. Thomas, P. S. Dale and R. Plomin, “Language impairment from 4 to 12 years: prediction and etiology,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. 850-865, 2014.
[14] J. B. Tomblin, N. L. Records, P. Buckwalter, P. Zhang, E. Smith and M. O’Brien, “Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 40, pp. 1245-1260, 1999.
[15] S. Reilly, B. Tomblin, J. Law, C. McKean, F. K. Mensah, A. Morgan, S. Goldfeld, J. M. Nicholson and M. Wake, “Specific language impairment: a convenient label for whom? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, vol. 49, no. 4, pp. 416–451, 2013.
[16] L. Baker and D. P. Cantwell, “A prospective psychiatric follow-up of children with speech/language disorders,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 26 no. 4, 546-553, 1987.
[17] M. C. St Clair, A. Pickles, K. Durkin and G. Conti-Ramsden, “A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI),” Journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 186-199, 2011.
[18] S. M. Redmond, A. C. Ash and T. P. Hogan, “Consequences of co-occurring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on children’s language impairments,” Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in School, vol. 46, pp. 68-80, 2015.
[19] S. M. Redmond, “Language impairment in the attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder context,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 59, pp. 133–142, 2016.
[20] Muller, K., & Tomblin B. J. (2012). Examining the comorbidity of language impairment and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Topics in Language Disorders, 32 (3), 228-246.
[21] J. H, Beitchman, J. Hood and A. Inglis, “Psychiatric risk in children with speech and language disorders,” Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 283-296, 1990.
[22] A. A. Benasich, S. Curtiss and P. Tallal, “Language, learning, and behavioral disturbances in childhood: a longitudinal perspective,” Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 585-594, 1993.
[23] G. Lindsay, J. E. Dockrell and S. Strand, “Longitudinal patterns of behaviour in children with specific speech and language difficulties: child and contextual factors,” British Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 77, no. 4, pp. 811-828, 2007.
[24] R. Tannock and R. Schachar, “Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,” Language, learning, and behavior disorders: developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives, vol. 38, pp. 128-155, 1996.
[25] P. A. Broen, “The verbal environment of the language learning child”, American Speech & Hearing Monographs, vol 17, pp. 134-145, 1972.
[26] A. J. Davis and B. K. Hathaway, “Reciprocity in parent-child verbal interactions,” The Journal of Genetic Psychology, vol. 140, pp. 169-183, 1982.
[27] C. Fraser and M. Roberts, “Mothers’ speech to children of four different ages,” Journal Psycholinguistics, vol. 4, pp. 9-16, 1975.
[28] A. Blackwell, S. Harding, S. Babayiğit and S. Roulstone, “Characteristics of parent-child interactions: a systematic review of studies comparing children with primary language impairment and their typically developing peers,” Communication Disorders Quarterly, vol. 36, pp. 67-78, 2015.
[29] F. Russell, “The expectations of parents of disabled children,” British Journal of Special Education, vol. 30, pp. 144-149, (2003.
[30] P. E. Davis-Kean, “The influence of parental education and family income on child achievement; the indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment,” Journal of Family Psychology, vol. 19 no.2, pp. 294-304, 2005.
[31] J. Allen and C. R. Marshall, “Parent-child interaction therapy in school-aged children with specific language impairment,” International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 397-410, 2011.
[32] P. J. Yoder and S. F. Warren, “Intentional communication elicits language-facilitating maternal responses in a dyads with children who have developmental disabilities,” American Journal of Mental Retardation, vol. 106, pp. 327-335, 2001.
[33] M. Siller and M. Sigman, “The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children's communication,” Journal of autism and developmental disorders, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 77-89, 2002.
[34] A. Fabrizi, A. Costa, L. Lucarelli and E. Patruno, “Comorbidity in specific language disorders and early feeding disorders: mother-child interactive patterns,” Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 152-160, 2010.
[35] M. Majorano and M. Lavelli, “Maternal input to children with specific language impairment during shared book reading: is mothers’ language in tune with their children's production,” International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 204-214, 2014.
[36] M. Breen and R. Barkley, “Child psychopathology and parenting stress in girls and boys having attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity,” Journal of Pediatric Psychology, vol. 13, 265–280, 1988.
[37] P. A. Graziano, J. P. McNamara, G. R. Geffken, and A. Reid, “Severity of children’s ADHD symptoms and parenting stress: a multiple mediation model of self-regulation,” Journal of abnormal child psychology, vol. 30, no. 7, 1073-1083, 2011.
[38] J. Johnson and Reader, “Assessing stress in families of children with ADHD: preliminary development of the disruptive behavior stress inventory (DBSI),” Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, vol. 9, pp. 51–62, 2002.
[39] E. J. Mash and Johnston, “A comparison of the mother-child interactions of physically abused and nonabused children during play and task situations,” Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, vol. 12, pp. 337–346, 1983.
[40] C. D. Strahm, Parents' Experience Raising a Child with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Ann Arbor, MI: ProQuest, 2008.
[41] V. Modesto-Lowe, J. S. Danforth, and D. Brooks, “ADHD: does parenting style matter?,” Clinical Pediatrics, vol. 47, no. 9, pp. 865-872, 2008.
[42] P. A. Graziano, J. P. McNamara, G. R. Geffken and A. M. Reid, “Differentiating co-occurring behavior problems in children with ADHD patterns of emotional reactivity and executive functioning,” Journal of attention disorders, vol. 17, pp. 249-260, 2013.
[43] C. R. Reynolds, R. W. Kamphaus, R. Wand K. J. Vannest, “Behavior assessment system for children (BASC),” In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology (pp. 366-371). New York: Springer, 2011.
[44] D. Wechsler, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Primary Scale of Intelligence - Fourth Edition (WPPSI™ - IV). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 2012.
[45] D. Shaffer, P. Fisher, M. K. Dulcan, M. Davies, J. Piacentini, M. E. Schwab-Stone and D. A. Regier, The NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA study,” Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 35, no. 7, pp. 865-877, 2000.
[46] I. E. Zimmerman, V. G. Steiner and R. E. Pond, Preschool Language Scales, Fifth Edition (PLS-5). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 2013.
[47] J. F. Miller and A. Iglesias, Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT), English & Spanish (Version 9) [Computer software]. Madison: University of Wisconsin Madison, Waisman Center, Language Analysis Laboratory, 2008.
[48] S. M. Redmond, H. L. Thompson and S. Goldstein, “Psycholinguistic profiling differentiates specific language impairment from typical development and from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 99-117, 201).
[49] K. Smith, “PLS-5 validity study: a comparison of test scores”, ASHA Community blog, 2014. http://community.asha.org/browse/blogs/blogviewer?BlogKey=b95c85ff-7cee-4d85-bfd8-807c1badb079&tab=recentcommunityblogsdashboard
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Eliane Ramos, Sisan Walker Angel, Katie Hart, Paulo Graziano. (2017). Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li. International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders, 1(1), 22-29. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Eliane Ramos; Sisan Walker Angel; Katie Hart; Paulo Graziano. Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li. Int. J. Commun. Sci. Disord. 2017, 1(1), 22-29. doi: 10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Eliane Ramos, Sisan Walker Angel, Katie Hart, Paulo Graziano. Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li. Int J Commun Sci Disord. 2017;1(1):22-29. doi: 10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15,
      author = {Eliane Ramos and Sisan Walker Angel and Katie Hart and Paulo Graziano},
      title = {Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li},
      journal = {International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders},
      volume = {1},
      number = {1},
      pages = {22-29},
      doi = {10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ijcsd.20170101.15},
      abstract = {This study examined the spontaneous language use between parents and their preschool children who are at high-risk for ADHD with a co-occurring language impairment (LI) and those without a co-occurring LI. Semantic and syntactic differences in language use were examined. Participants consisted of 20 children ages three to five years old and their parents. For each parent-child dyad, a 15-minute video-recorded interaction was orthographically transcribed and analyzed using the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software program. Children with co-occuring LI showed significantly lower Mean Length of Utterance (MLU), and Type-Token Ratios (TTR). Both groups showed very limited use of complex sentences and no specific difficulties with grammatical morphemes. Parents in both groups did not seem to adapt their language level whether or not their children had a LI.},
     year = {2017}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Spontaneous Language Use by Parents and Their Preschool Children at Risk for Adhd with or Without Li
    AU  - Eliane Ramos
    AU  - Sisan Walker Angel
    AU  - Katie Hart
    AU  - Paulo Graziano
    Y1  - 2017/06/13
    PY  - 2017
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15
    DO  - 10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15
    T2  - International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders
    JF  - International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders
    JO  - International Journal of Communication Sciences and Disorders
    SP  - 22
    EP  - 29
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijcsd.20170101.15
    AB  - This study examined the spontaneous language use between parents and their preschool children who are at high-risk for ADHD with a co-occurring language impairment (LI) and those without a co-occurring LI. Semantic and syntactic differences in language use were examined. Participants consisted of 20 children ages three to five years old and their parents. For each parent-child dyad, a 15-minute video-recorded interaction was orthographically transcribed and analyzed using the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software program. Children with co-occuring LI showed significantly lower Mean Length of Utterance (MLU), and Type-Token Ratios (TTR). Both groups showed very limited use of complex sentences and no specific difficulties with grammatical morphemes. Parents in both groups did not seem to adapt their language level whether or not their children had a LI.
    VL  - 1
    IS  - 1
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA

  • Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA

  • Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA

  • Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA

  • Sections